Some Challenges for Research on Emotion and Moral Judgment: The Moral Foreign-Language Effect as a Case Study

Main Article Content

Steven McFarlane
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0199-7880
Heather Cipolletti Perez
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1159-859X

Abstract

In this article, we discuss a number of challenges with the empirical study of emotion and its relation to moral judgment. We examine a case study involving the moral foreign-language effect, according to which people show an increased utilitarian response tendency in moral dilemmas when using their non-native language. One important proposed explanation for this effect is that using one’s non-native language reduces emotional arousal, and that reduced emotion is responsible for this tendency. We offer reasons to think that there is insufficient evidence for accepting this explanation at present. We argue that there are three themes that constrain our current ability to draw firm empirical conclusions: 1) the frequent use of proxies or partial measures for emotions, 2) the lack of a predictive and generalizable theory of emotion and specific emotion-types, and 3) the obscurity of a baseline level of neutrality with respect to participant emotion. These lessons apply not only to research on the moral foreign-language effect, but to empirical research in moral psychology more generally. 

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Article Details

How to Cite
McFarlane, Steven, and Heather Cipolletti Perez. 2020. “Some Challenges for Research on Emotion and Moral Judgment: The Moral Foreign-Language Effect As a Case Study”. Diametros 17 (64):56-71. https://doi.org/10.33392/diam.1476.
Section
Articles
Share |

References

Bago B., De Neys W. (2019), “The Intuitive Greater Good: Testing the Corrective Dual Process Model of Moral Cognition,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 148 (10): 1782–1801.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000533

Baron J., Gürçay B., Luce M.F. (2018), “Correlations of Trait and State Emotions with Utilitarian Moral Judgements,” Cognition and Emotion 32 (1): 116–129.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2017.1295025

Batson C.D., Coke J.S., Chard F. et al (1979), “Generality of the ‘Glow of Goodwill’: Effectsof Mood on Helping and Information Acquisition,” Social Psychology Quarterly 42 (2): 176–179.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.2307/3033698

Białek M., De Neys W. (2016), “Conflict Detection During Moral Decision-Making: Evidence for Deontic Reasoners’ Utilitarian Sensitivity,” Journal of Cognitive Psychology 28 (5): 631–639.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2016.1156118

Białek M., De Neys W. (2017), “Dual Processes and Moral Conflict: Evidence for Deontological Reasoners’ Intuitive Utilitarian Sensitivity,” Judgment and Decision Making 12 (2): 148–167.

Białek M., Paruzel-Czachura M., Gawronski B. (2019), “Foreign Language Effects on Moral Dilemma Judgments: An Analysis Using the CNI Model,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 85: 103855.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2019.103855

Blevins G.A., Murphy T. (1974), “Feeling Good and Helping: Further Phonebooth Findings,” Psychological Reports 34 (1): 326.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1974.34.1.326

Bond M.H., Lai T.M. (1986), “Embarrassment and Code-Switching Into a Second Language,” Journal of Social Psychology 126 (2): 179–186.

Caldwell-Harris C.L., Ayçiçeği-Dinn A. (2009), “Emotion and Lying in a Non-Native Language,” International Journal of Psychophysiology 71 (3): 193–204.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.09.006

Choe S.Y., Min K. (2011), “Who Makes Utilitarian Judgments? The Influences of Emotions on Utilitarian Judgments,” Judgment and Decision Making 6 (7): 580–592.

Cipolletti, H., McFarlane, S., Weissglass, C. (2016), “The Moral Foreign-Language Effect,” Philosophical Psychology 29 (1): 23–40.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2014.993063

Colbeck K.L., Bowers J.S. (2012), “Blinded by Taboo Words in L1 but not L2,” Emotion 12 (2): 217–222.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026387

Corey J.D., Hayakawa S., Foucart A. et al. (2017), “Our Moral Choices are Foreign to Us,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 43 (7): 1109–1128.

Costa A., Foucart A., Hayakawa S. et al. (2014), “Your Morals Depend on Your Language,” PLoS One 9 (4): e94842.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094842

De Langhe B., Puntoni S., Fernandes D. et al. (2011). “The Anchor Contraction Effect in International Marketing Research,” Journal of Marketing Research 48 (2): 366–380.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.48.2.366

Dewaele J.M. (2010), Emotions in Multiple Languages, Palgrave Macmillan, Basingstoke, England.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1057/9780230289505

Drake B. (2012), “To Avoid Stupid Mistakes,” Bloomberg Report, URL = https://www.bloomberg.com/news/articles/2012-04-25/to-avoid-stupid-mistakes-think-in-french [Accessed 13.02.2020].

Gawinkowska M., Paradowski M.B., Bilewicz M. (2013), “Second Language as an Exemptor from Sociocultural Norms: Emotion-Related Language Choice Revisited,” PloS One 8 (12): e81225.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0081225

Gawronski B., Armstrong J., Conway P. et al. (2017), “Consequences, Norms, and Generalized Inaction in Moral Dilemmas: The CNI Model of Moral Decision-Making,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 113 (3): 343–376.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000086

Gawronski B., Conway P., Armstrong J. et al. (2018), “Effects of Incidental Emotions on Moral Dilemma Judgments: An Analysis Using the CNI Model,” Emotion 18 (7): 989–1008.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000399

Geipel J., Hadjichristidis C., Surian L. (2015), “The Foreign Language Effect on Moral Judgement: The Role of Emotions and Norms,” PLoS One 10 (7): e0131529.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131529

Geipel J., Hadjichristidis C., Surian, L. (2016), “Foreign Language Affects the Contribution of Intentions and Outcomes to Moral Judgment,” Cognition 154: 34–39.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.05.010

Greene J.D., Sommerville R.B., Nystrom L.E. et al. (2001), “An fMRI Investigation of Emotional Engagement in Moral Judgment,” Science 293 (5537): 2105–2108.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1062872

Greene J.D., Sommerville R.B., Nystrom L.E. et al. (2008), “The Secret Joke of Kant’s Soul” [in:] Moral Psychology Vol. 3: The Neuroscience of Morality: Emotion, Disease, and Development, W. Sinnott-Armstrong (ed.), The MIT Press, Cambridge (MA): 35–79.

Greene J.D., Sommerville R.B., Nystrom L.E. et al. (2013), Moral Tribes: Emotion, Reason, and the Gap Between Us and Them, Penguin Books, New York.

Greene R.L. (2012), “Oprima Dos for Better Cognition,” The Economist, URL = https://www.economist.com/johnson/2012/05/08/oprima-dos-for-better-cognition [Accessed 13.02.2020].

Gross J. (2012), “Reasoning is Sharper in a Foreign Language,” Scientific American, URL = https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/reasoning-is-sharper-in-a-foreign-language [Accessed 13.02.2020].
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamericanmind1112-7a

Haidt J. (2001), “The Emotional Dog and Its Rational Tail: A Social Intuitionist Approach to Moral Judgment,” Psychological Review 108 (4): 814–834.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.4.814

Harris C. L., Ayçiçeği A., Gleason J.B. (2003), “Taboo Words and Reprimands Elicit Greater Autonomic Reactivity in a First than in a Second Language,” Applied Psycholinguistics 24 (04): 561–579.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716403000286

Harris C.L., Ayçiçeği-Dinn A., Gleason J.B. (2006), “When is a First Language More Emotional? Psychophysiological Evidence from Bilingual Speakers”, [in:] Bilingual Minds: Emotional Experience, Expression, and Representation, A. Pavlenko (ed.), Multilingual Matters, Clevedon, England: 257– 283.

Hayakawa S., Tannebaum D., Costa A. et al. (2017), “Thinking More or Feeling Less? Explaining the Foreign-Language Effect on Moral Judgment,” Psychological Science 28 (10): 1387–1397.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617720944

Horne Z., Powell D. (2016), “How Large Is the Role of Emotion in Judgments of Moral Dilemmas?”, PLoS ONE 11 (7): e0154780.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0154780

Kahane G., Shackel N. (2008), “Do Abnormal Responses Show Utilitarian Bias?,” Nature 452: E5.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06785

Kahane G., Shackel N. (2010), “Methodological Issues in the Neuroscience of Moral Judgement,” Mind & Language 25 (5): 561–582.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2010.01401.x

Kahane G., Wiech K., Shackel N. et al. (2011), “The Neural Basis of Intuitive and Counterintuitive Moral Judgment,” Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 7 (4): 393–402.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr005

Kahneman D. (2013), Thinking, Fast and Slow, Farrar, Straus and Giroux, New York.

Keysar B., Hayakawa S., An S. (2012), “The Foreign-Language Effect: Thinking in a Foreign Tongue Reduces Decision Biases,” Psychological Science 23 (6): 661–668.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611432178

Landy J.F., Goodwin G.P. (2015). “Does Incidental Disgust Amplify Moral Judgment? A Meta-Analytic Review of Experimental Evidence,” Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10 (4): 518–536.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615583128

Lerner J.S., Li Y., Valdesolo P. et al. (2015), “Emotion and Decision Making,” Annual Review of Psychology 66: 799–823.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115043

Lieberman M. (2017), “Thinking in a Second Language Makes You More Logical,” Travel & Leisure, URL = https://www.travelandleisure.com/travel-tips/languages-affect-our-rationality-and-morality [Accessed 13.02.2020].

Muda R., Niszczota P., Białek M. et al. (2018), “Reading Dilemmas in a Foreign Language Reduces Both Deontological and Utilitarian Response Tendencies,” Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition 44 (2): 321–326.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000447

Pfister H., Böhm G. (2008), “The Multiplicity of Emotions: A Framework of Emotional Functions in Decision Making,” Judgment and Decision Making 3 (1): 5–17.

Reynolds C.J., Conway P. (2018), “Not Just Bad Actions: Affective Concern for Bad Outcomes Contributes to Moral Condemnation of Harm in Moral Dilemmas,” Emotion 18 (7): 1009–1023.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000413

Schnall S., Haidt J., Clore G.L. et al. (2008), “Disgust as Embodied Moral Judgment,” Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin 34 (8): 1096–109.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167208317771

Schnall S., Haidt J., Clore G.L. et al. (2015), “Landy and Goodwin (2015) Confirmed Most of Our Findings Then Drew the Wrong Conclusions,” Perspectives on Psychological Science 10 (4): 537–538.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615589078

Schwarz N., Bless H. (1991), “Happy and Mindless, but Sad and Smart? The Impact of Affective States on Analytic Reasoning,” [in:] Emotion and Social Judgments, J.P. Forgas (ed.), Pergamon Press, Elmsford, NY: 55–71.

Sevinc G., Spreng R.N. (2014), “Contextual and Perceptual Brain Processes Underlying Moral Cognition: A Quantitative Meta-Analysis of Moral Reasoning and Moral Emotions,” PLoS ONE 9 (2): e87427.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0087427

Valdesolo P., DeSteno D. (2006), “Manipulations of Emotional Context Shape Moral Judgement,” Psychological Science 17: 476–477.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01731.x

Vives M., Aparici M., Costa A. (2018), “The Limits of the Foreign Language Effect on Decision-Making: The Case of Outcome Bias and the Representativeness Heuristic,” PLoS ONE 13 (9): e0203528.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203528

Weyant J., Clark R.D. (1976), “Dimes and Helping: The Other Side of the Coin,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 3 (1): 107 –110.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/014616727600300119